Rocksolid Light

Welcome to novaBBS (click a section below)

mail  files  register  newsreader  groups  login

Message-ID:  

6 May, 2024: The networking issue during the past two days has been identified and appears to be fixed. Will keep monitoring.


interests / soc.history.medieval / Can Progressives Be Convinced That Genetics Matters?

Can Progressives Be Convinced That Genetics Matters?

<4_y%I.23115$rsCb.22488@fx01.iad>

  copy mid

https://www.novabbs.com/interests/article-flat.php?id=1493&group=soc.history.medieval#1493

  copy link   Newsgroups: soc.history.medieval
Path: i2pn2.org!i2pn.org!usenet.goja.nl.eu.org!3.eu.feeder.erje.net!feeder.erje.net!feeder1.feed.usenet.farm!feed.usenet.farm!news.uzoreto.com!news-out.netnews.com!news.alt.net!fdc2.netnews.com!peer01.ams1!peer.ams1.xlned.com!news.xlned.com!peer02.iad!feed-me.highwinds-media.com!news.highwinds-media.com!fx01.iad.POSTED!not-for-mail
MIME-Version: 1.0
User-Agent: Mozilla/5.0 (Windows NT 10.0; Win64; x64; rv:91.0) Gecko/20100101
Thunderbird/91.0.3
Newsgroups: soc.history.medieval
Content-Language: en-US
From: a425cou...@hotmail.com (a425couple)
Subject: Can Progressives Be Convinced That Genetics Matters?
Content-Type: text/plain; charset=UTF-8; format=flowed
Content-Transfer-Encoding: 8bit
Lines: 996
Message-ID: <4_y%I.23115$rsCb.22488@fx01.iad>
X-Complaints-To: abuse(at)newshosting.com
NNTP-Posting-Date: Mon, 13 Sep 2021 02:48:32 UTC
Organization: Newshosting.com - Highest quality at a great price! www.newshosting.com
Date: Sun, 12 Sep 2021 19:48:34 -0700
X-Received-Bytes: 64878
 by: a425couple - Mon, 13 Sep 2021 02:48 UTC

From The New Yorker, a quite liberal publication,
but open to contradictions.
It is long, but interesting.

from
https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2021/09/13/can-progressives-be-convinced-that-genetics-matters

Profiles
September 13, 2021 Issue
(this was Published in the print edition of the September 13,
2021, issue, with the headline “Force of Nature.”)

Can Progressives Be Convinced That Genetics Matters?
The behavior geneticist Kathryn Paige Harden is waging a two-front
campaign: on her left are those who assume that genes are irrelevant, on
her right those who insist that they’re everything.
By Gideon Lewis-Kraus

September 6, 2021
Kathryn Paige Harden
“Building a commitment to egalitarianism on our genetic uniformity is
building a house on sand,” Harden writes.
Photograph by Dan Winters for The New Yorker

Until she was thirty-three, Kathryn Paige Harden, a professor of
psychology at the University of Texas at Austin, had enjoyed a
vocational ascent so steady that it seemed guided by the hand of
predestination. When she first went on the job market, at twenty-six,
her graduate-school mentor, Eric Turkheimer, a professor at the
University of Virginia, recommended her with an almost mystified
alacrity. “More than anyone else who has come through my lab, I find
myself answering questions by saying, ‘We should check with Paige,’ ” he
wrote. “I am absolutely confident she will be a successful addition to
any faculty, and she brings a significant chance of being a superstar.”
Her early scholarship was singled out for prestigious awards and grants,
and she was offered tenure at thirty-two. In 2016, she began co-hosting
an Introduction to Psychology class from a soundstage, in the style of a
morning show—she and her colleague drank coffee from matching mugs—that
was live-streamed each semester to more than a thousand students. She
couldn’t cross campus without being stopped for selfies.

Harden works in the field of behavior genetics, which investigates the
influence of genes on character traits (neuroticism, agreeableness) and
life outcomes (educational attainment, income, criminality). Such
research has historically relied upon “twin studies,” which compare
identical twins with fraternal ones to differentiate genetic from
environmental effects. As a new professor, she co-founded the Texas Twin
Project, the first registry engineered to maximize representation of
low-income families from ethnically diverse backgrounds. In a recent
paper, Harden asked, “You only have one life to live, but if you rewound
the tape and started anew from the exact same genetic and environmental
starting point, how differently could your life go?” She continued,
“Overall, twin research suggests that, in your alternate life, you might
not have gotten divorced, you might have made more money, you might be
more extraverted or organized—but you are unlikely to be substantially
different in your cognitive ability, education, or mental disease.” In
the past few years, Harden noted, new molecular techniques have begun to
shore up the basic finding that our personal trajectories owe a
considerable debt to our genes.

On sabbatical for the 2015-16 academic year, Harden and Elliot
Tucker-Drob, a colleague to whom she was married at the time, were
invited to New York City with their two young children—a three-year-old
boy and a nine-month-old girl—as visiting scholars-in-residence at the
Russell Sage Foundation. Russell Sage, which occupies a handsome Philip
Johnson building in Manhattan, primarily supports sociologists,
journalists, and economists, but it had recently launched an initiative
to integrate the biological sciences. Harden felt almost immediately
unwelcome at the regular fellows’ lunches. Many of the left-leaning
social scientists seemed certain that behavior-genetics research, no
matter how well intentioned, was likely to lead us down the garden path
to eugenics. The world would be better, Harden was told, if she quit.
When their cohort went to see “Hamilton,” the others professed surprise
that Harden and Tucker-Drob had enjoyed it, as if their work could be
done only by people uncomfortable with an inclusive vision of American
history.

Harden assumed that such leeriness was the vestige of a bygone era, when
genes were described as the “hard-wiring” of individual fate, and that
her critics might be reassured by updated information. Two weeks before
her family was due to return to Texas, she e-mailed the fellows a new
study, in Psychological Science, led by Daniel Belsky, at Duke. The
paper drew upon a major international collaboration that had identified
sites on the genome that evinced a statistically significant correlation
with educational attainment; Belsky and his colleagues used that data to
compile a “polygenic score”—a weighted sum of an individual’s relevant
genetic variants—that could partly explain population variance in
reading ability and years of schooling. His study sampled New Zealanders
of northern-European descent and was carefully controlled for childhood
socioeconomic status. “Hope that you find this interesting food for
thought,” she wrote.

William Darity, a professor of public policy at Duke and perhaps the
country’s leading scholar on the economics of racial inequality,
answered curtly, starting a long chain of replies. Given the
difficulties of distinguishing between genetic and environmental effects
on social outcomes, he wrote, such investigations were at best futile:
“There will be no reason to pursue these types of research programs at
all, and they can be rendered to the same location as Holocaust denial
research.” By the time he wrote again, several hours later, one of
Harden’s few supporters among the fellows had changed the thread’s
subject line from “new genetics paper” to “Seriously? Holocaust
deniers?” Darity responded, “I feel just as strongly that we should not
keep the notions that the world is 6000 years old or that climate change
is a fabrication under consideration.”

Harden remarked that being called a climate skeptic was marginally
preferable to being called a Holocaust denier. She offered to host a
lunch to discuss the uncontroversial basics of genetics research for
anyone interested. Darity was reluctant to let the matter go: “One final
comment from me, and then I will withdraw into my pique.” In 1994, he
wrote, the political scientist Charles Murray and the late psychologist
Richard Herrnstein “published a bestseller that achieved great
notoriety, The Bell Curve. Apart from its claims about a genetic basis
for a ‘racial’ hierarchy in intelligence, the book claimed that social
outcomes like poverty and inequality in earnings had a genetic
foundation. Personally, I thought the book was outrageous and a
saddening resuscitation of ideas that had increasingly been dismissed as
‘pseudoscience.’ Belsky’s work strikes me as an extension of the
Murray-Herrnstein view of the world.” He concluded, “At some point, I
think we need to say enough is enough.” (Darity told me, of his e-mails,
“I stand by all that.”)

An admirer of Darity’s work—especially on reparations for slavery—Harden
was surprised that she’d elicited such rancor from someone with whom she
was otherwise in near-total political agreement. In the wake of the
exchange, some of the other fellows stopped speaking to Harden, and the
e-mail chain was forwarded to members of the foundation’s board. The
next year, after winning the American Psychological Association’s
Distinguished Scientific Award for an Early Career Contribution to
Psychology, Harden applied for a grant from Russell Sage’s biosciences
initiative, which had supported similar research in the past. She
received enthusiastic peer reviews from its scientific advisers, and was
given to understand that the grant’s disbursal was a fait accompli.
During a contentious meeting, however, the full board voted to overturn
the scientific panel’s recommendation. Over the next year, a biosciences
working group revised the program’s funding guidelines, stipulating in
the final draft that it would not support any research into the
first-order effects of genes on behavior or social outcomes. In the end,
the board chose to disband the initiative entirely. (A spokesperson for
Russell Sage told me by e-mail that the decision was based on the
“consideration of numerous factors, including RSF’s relative lack of
expertise in this area.”)

Harden has spent the last five years thinking about Darity’s objections.
As she put it to me recently, “When I reread his e-mails, it all struck
me as very Chekhovian. Like, here are all the guns that are going to go
off in Act V.” Harden understands why the left, with which she
identifies, has nurtured an aversion to genetics. She went to graduate
school in Charlottesville, the birthplace of Carrie Buck, a
“feeble-minded” woman who was sterilized against her will, in 1927,
under a state eugenics program sanctioned by the Supreme Court. But she
does not believe that a recognition of this horrifying history ought to
entail the peremptory rejection of the current scientific consensus. The
left’s decision to withdraw from conversations about genetics and social
outcomes leaves a vacuum that the right has gaily filled. The situation
has been exploited as a “red pill” to expose liberal hypocrisy. Today,
Harden is at the forefront of an inchoate movement, sometimes referred
to as the “hereditarian left,” dedicated to the development of a new
moral framework for talking about genetics.

ADVERTISEMENT

This fall, Princeton University Press will publish Harden’s book, “The
Genetic Lottery: Why DNA Matters for Social Equality,” which attempts to
reconcile the findings of her field with her commitments to social
justice. As she writes, “Yes, the genetic differences between any two
people are tiny when compared to the long stretches of DNA coiled in
every human cell. But these differences loom large when trying to
understand why, for example, one child has autism and another doesn’t;
why one is deaf and another hearing; and—as I will describe in this
book—why one child will struggle with school and another will not.
Genetic differences between us matter for our lives. They cause
differences in things we care about. Building a commitment to
egalitarianism on our genetic uniformity is building a house on sand.”

Harden understands herself to be waging a two-front campaign. On her
left are those inclined to insist that genes don’t really matter; on her
right are those who suspect that genes are, in fact, the only things
that matter. The history of behavior genetics is the story of each
generation’s attempt to chart a middle course. When the discipline first
began to coalesce, in the early nineteen-sixties, the memory of Nazi
atrocities rendered the eugenics threat distinctly untheoretical. The
reigning model of human development, which seemed to accord with postwar
liberal principles, was behaviorism, with its hope that environmental
manipulation could produce any desired outcome. It did not take much,
however, to notice that there is considerable variance in the
distribution of human abilities. The early behavior geneticists started
with the premise that our nature is neither perfectly fixed nor
perfectly plastic, and that this was a good thing. They conscripted as
their intellectual patriarch the Russian émigré Theodosius Dobzhansky,
an evolutionary biologist who was committed to anti-racism and to the
conviction that “genetic diversity is mankind’s most precious resource,
not a regrettable deviation from an ideal state of monotonous sameness.”

VIDEO FROM THE NEW YORKER

Re-creating the Syria of His Memories, Through Miniatures

The field’s modern pioneers were keen to establish that their interest
lay in academic questions, and they prioritized the comparatively
clement study of animals. In 1965, John Paul Scott and John L. Fuller
reported that, despite the discernible genetic differences among dog
breeds, there did not seem to be categorical distinctions that might
allow one to conclude that, say, German shepherds were smarter than
Labradors. The most important variations occurred on an individual
level, and environmental conditions were as important as innate
qualities, if not more so.

This era of comity did not last long. In 1969, Arthur Jensen, a
respected psychologist at Berkeley, published an article called “How
Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement?” in the Harvard
Educational Review. Jensen coolly argued that there was an I.Q. gap
between the races in America; that the reason for this gap was at least
partly genetic, and thus, unfortunately, immutable; and that policy
interventions were unlikely to thwart the natural hierarchy. The Jensen
affair, which extended for more than a decade, prefigured the
publication of “The Bell Curve”: endless public debate, student
protests, burned effigies, death threats, accusations of intellectual
totalitarianism. As Aaron Panofsky writes in “Misbehaving Science,” a
history of the discipline, “Controversies wax and wane, sometimes they
emerge explosively, but they never really resolve and always threaten to
reappear.”

The problem was that most of Jensen’s colleagues agreed with some of his
basic claims: it did seem that there was something akin to “general
intelligence” in humans, that it could be meaningfully measured with
I.Q. tests, and that genetic inheritance has a good deal to do with it.
Critics quickly pointed out that the convoluted social pathways that led
from genes to complex traits rendered any simple notion of genetic
“causation” silly. In 1972, Christopher Jencks, a sociologist at
Harvard, proposed the thought experiment of a country in which
red-haired children were prevented from going to school. One might
anticipate that such children would demonstrate a weaker reading
ability, which, because red hair is genetic in origin, would be
conspicuously linked to their genes—and would, in some bizarre sense, be
“caused” by them.

Richard Lewontin, a geneticist and a staunch egalitarian, developed a
different analogy. Imagine a bag of seed corn. If you plant one handful
in nutrient-poor soil, and another in rich loam, there will be a stark
difference in their average stalk height, irrespective of any genetic
predisposition. (There will also be greater “inequality” among the
well-provisioned plants; perhaps counterintuitively, the more uniformly
beneficial the climate, the more pronounced the effects of genetic
difference.) Jensen’s racial comparison was thus unwarranted and
invidious: it was absurd to think, in the America of 1969, that
different races enjoyed equally bountiful circumstances.

TITLE Great CrowdSurf Misjudgments
Cartoon by Paul Noth
Behavior geneticists emphasized that their own studies showed that
poorer children adopted by wealthy families saw substantial gains in
average I.Q. This finding, it later emerged, obtained on a societal
basis as well. The scholar James Flynn found that, for reasons that are
not entirely understood, the average I.Q. of a population increases
significantly over time: most people living a hundred years ago, were
they given contemporary I.Q. tests, would easily have qualified as what
early psychometricians called, with putative technical precision,
“morons” or “imbeciles.” Such tests might be measuring something real,
but whatever it is cannot be considered “purely” biological or inflexible.

Our ability to remediate genetic differences was thus a separate moral
question. In 1979, the economist Arthur Goldberger published a mordant
rejoinder to social conservatives who argued that genetic differences
rendered the welfare apparatus supererogatory. “In the same vein, if it
were shown that a large proportion of the variance in eyesight were due
to genetic causes, then the Royal Commission on the Distribution of
Eyeglasses might well pack up,” he wrote. Just because outcomes might be
partly genetic didn’t mean that they were inevitable.

As twin studies proliferated throughout the nineteen-eighties, their
results contributed to substantial changes in our moral intuitions. When
schizophrenia and autism, for example, turned out to be largely
heritable, we no longer blamed these disorders on cold or inept mothers.
But, for such freighted traits as intelligence, liberals remained
understandably anxious and continued to insist that differences—not just
on a group level but on an individual one—were merely artifacts of an
unequal environment. Conservatives pointed out that an à-la-carte
approach to scientific findings was intellectually incoherent.

In 1997, Turkheimer, perhaps the preëminent behavior geneticist of his
generation, published a short political meditation called “The Search
for a Psychometric Left,” in which he called upon his fellow-liberals to
accept that they had nothing to fear from genes. He proposed that “a
psychometric left would recognize that human ability, individual
differences in human ability, measures of human ability, and genetic
influences on human ability are all real but profoundly complex, too
complex for the imposition of biogenetic or political schemata. It would
assert that the most important difference between the races is racism,
with its origins in the horrific institution of slavery only a very few
generations ago. Opposition to determinism, reductionism and racism, in
their extreme or moderate forms, need not depend on blanket rejection of
undeniable if easily misinterpreted facts like heritability.” He
concluded, “Indeed it had better not, because if it does the eventual
victory of the psychometric right is assured.”

Having endured the summer of 2020 trapped indoors in the oppressive
Austin heat, Harden was grateful for an invitation to spend this past
June at Montana State University, in Bozeman. A recent influx of
out-of-town wealth had accelerated during the pandemic, and the town’s
industrial fixtures had been ruthlessly spruced up to suit the needs of
remote knowledge workers. Harden, who has moss-colored eyes, a wry
smile, and an earnest nonchalance, met me at a coffee shop that looked
as though it had been airlifted that morning from San Francisco. She
wore a soft flannel shirt, faded stone-washed jeans, and dark Ray-Ban
sunglasses. The air was hot and dry, but Harden is the sort of person
who seems accompanied by a perpetual breeze. “ ‘The Bell Curve’ came out
when I was twelve years old, and somehow that’s still what people are
talking about,” she said. “There’s a new white dude in every generation
who gets famous talking about this.” Virtually every time Harden gives a
presentation, someone asks about “Gattaca,” the 1997 movie about a
dystopia structured by genetic caste. Harden responds that the life of a
behavior geneticist resembles a different nineties classic: “Groundhog Day.”

Harden was raised in a conservative environment, and though she later
rejected much of her upbringing, she has maintained a convert’s distrust
of orthodoxy. Her father’s family were farmers and pipeline workers in
Texas, and her grandparents—Pentecostalists who embraced faith healing
and speaking in tongues—were lifted out of extreme poverty by the
military. “It was the classic tale of the government’s deliberate
creation of a white middle class,” she said. Her father served as a Navy
pilot, then took a job flying for FedEx, and Harden and her brother grew
up in an exurb of Memphis. Harden scandalized her Christian high school
when, at fifteen, she wrote a term paper about “The Bell Jar.” She has
not recapitulated the arc of her parents’ lives. “They’re still very
religious—very suspicious of the mainstream media, secular universities,
secular anything, which has accelerated in the Trump years.”

Harden’s parents insisted that she stay in the South for college, and
Furman University, a formerly Baptist college in South Carolina, gave
her a full scholarship based on her near-perfect SAT scores. She
received paid summer fellowships in rodent genetics, and found that she
preferred the grunt work of the lab bench to the difficult multitasking
required by the jobs in waitressing and retail to which she was
accustomed. She only later realized that the point of the program was to
draw students from underrepresented backgrounds into science. At twenty,
she applied to graduate school in clinical psychology. Her father’s only
comment was “I was afraid you were going to say that.” She was rejected
almost everywhere, but Turkheimer, noting her lab experience and her
exceptionally high quantitative G.R.E. scores, invited her for an
interview. She wore a new Ann Taylor suit and he wore Tevas.
Turkheimer’s e-mail avatar is the Greek letter psi, for “psychology,”
set against the Grateful Dead logo; he offered her admission on the
condition that she stop calling him “sir.”

Her experiences as an apprentice scientist were only part of the reason
that she grew disillusioned with evangelicalism: “There was this
incredible post-9/11 nationalism—flags on the altar next to crosses—that
infected my church to a point that felt immoral and gross. Sometimes I
feel like I sat through eleven years of Christian school and absorbed
all the things they didn’t intend for me to absorb. I thought we were
following a social-justice ethos in which the meek shall inherit the
earth, and I must’ve missed the track that was the run-up to the Iraq
War.” Turkheimer recommended a local psychoanalyst, who, Harden said,
took her on as a “charity case.”

It might have seemed peculiar that a behavior geneticist was
recommending analytic treatment, but Turkheimer had long been known for
his belief that biological explanations for behavior were unlikely ever
to supplant cultural and psychological ones. Turkheimer’s longtime
rival, the prolific researcher Robert Plomin, believed otherwise,
predicting that we would one day achieve molecular-level purchase on
what makes people who they are. Turkheimer associated himself with what
Plomin lamented as “the gloomy prospect”—the notion that the relevant
processes were too messy and idiosyncratic to be fixed under glass. The
prospect was gloomy, Turkheimer said, only from the perspective of a
social scientist. As a person, he had a more sanguine view: “In the long
run, the gloomy prospect always wins, and no one would want to live in a
world where it did not.”

Woman bakes unappreciative man a birthday cake.
“I’m not undermining your diet. I’m baking you a birthday cake.”
Cartoon by William Haefeli
This did not mean that behavior genetics was useless, only that it
required a modest perspective on what could be achieved: twin studies
might never explain how a given genotype made someone more likely to be
depressed, but they could help avoid the kind of mistaken inference that
blamed bad parenting. Harden’s work in Turkheimer’s lab remained
squarely within this tradition. For example, the state of Texas spent a
lot of money on school programs to promote sexual abstinence, on the
basis of research that showed a correlation between adolescent sexuality
and subsequent antisocial behavior. Harden used a twin study to
demonstrate that a twin who began having sex early showed no greater
likelihood of engaging in risky behavior than her twin who had
abstained. In other words, both behaviors might be the expression of
some underlying predisposition, but no causal arrow could be drawn. She
did similar work to show that the idea of “peer pressure” as a driver of
adolescent substance abuse was, at best, a radical oversimplification of
an extremely complex transactional dynamic between genes and environment.

ADVERTISEMENT

Harden’s years in graduate school coincided with the arrival of actual
geneticists in a field long dominated by psychologists. In 2003,
scientists completed the first full map of the human genome, and it
seemed as though Plomin’s vision would be borne out. Some
illnesses—Huntington’s, for example—turned out to be the result of a
mutation in a single gene, and there was a widespread assumption that
complex personality traits might be as cleanly derived. A gene was
purportedly identified for aggression, and one for depression, and one
for homosexuality. But these studies couldn’t be replicated, and the
“candidate gene” era had to be written off as a gross misstep. It became
clear that complex traits were governed by multiple genes, and that
individual genes could pertain to a variety of attributes.

Around the time that Harden was finishing her dissertation, however,
researchers began to wonder if it might be possible to identify hundreds
or even thousands of places in the genome where differences in our DNA
sequences could be correlated with a trait or an outcome. This research
design was called a “genome-wide association study,” or gwas (pronounced
ji-wass). Turkheimer was characteristically unimpressed with the initial
results, which were weak. At the annual conference of the Behavior
Genetics Association in 2013, he delivered a withering keynote address:
trying to understand human behavior with a gwas was like putting a CD
under a microscope to figure out if a song was good. Harden, too, was
sure that they would not learn anything from these contrived statistical
exercises. “But we were wrong,” she said.

In the last five years, gwas results have rapidly evolved. Polygenic
scores can now account for a good deal of a population’s variance in
height and weight, and have been shown to predict cardiovascular disease
and diabetes. “This is really a cause for celebration,” Plomin told me.
“Imagine the advent of predictive medicine—to be able to identify
medical issues before they occur.” Researchers have also found links
with complex behavioral traits. “Significant hits have been reported for
traits such as coffee and tea consumption, chronic sleep disturbances
(insomnia), tiredness, and even whether an individual is a morning
person or a night person,” Plomin notes, in his 2018 book, “Blueprint:
How DNA Makes Us Who We Are.” The new research, he writes, “signals the
start of the DNA revolution in psychology.”

The largest gwas for educational attainment to date found almost
thirteen hundred sites on the genome that are correlated with success in
school. Though each might have an infinitesimally small statistical
relationship with the outcome, together they can be summed to produce a
score that has predictive validity: those in the group with the highest
scores were approximately five times more likely to graduate from
college than those with the lowest scores—about as accurate a predictor
as traditional social-science variables like parental income. Nobody
knows quite what to do with these results, but, as one population
geneticist put it to me, “the train has left the station—even if
researchers don’t fully understand what they’re learning, this is how
the genome is used now.”

Harden and her collaborators currently conduct their own gwas efforts;
most recently, they have investigated behaviors including adolescent
aggression and risktaking, which are strongly predictive of life span
and labor-market outcomes. She knows that she may never convince
Turkheimer, who continues to argue that the light these studies generate
is too faint to dispel his gloom. But she thinks that they represent an
incremental step forward: “Eric says it’s dangerous to talk about genes
if you don’t know exactly how they’re associated with the outcome, but
we don’t even really know how, exactly, poverty changes things—why is it
good to be adopted into a rich family?” She added, “It’s impossible for
me not to care about how what people start with shapes their lives.”

Harden was joined in Bozeman by her younger brother, Micah, who was
visiting from Memphis. We sat together on the covered patio of the airy
house Harden had rented with her boyfriend, an architectural designer
named Travis Avery. It was the longest spell she had ever spent away
from her children, who were on a road trip with Tucker-Drob. (The couple
got divorced in 2018.) Micah had not yet read his sister’s book but had
grudgingly agreed to be genotyped for it. “We have the same brown hair,
same green eyes, same tendency to do what our stepmother refers to as
the ‘Harden slow-blink,’ closing our eyes for a few seconds when we are
annoyed at someone,” she writes. “Despite these similarities, our lives
have turned out differently.” Micah still lives near their childhood
home, has not left the church, and can run up and down a soccer field
“without gasping for oxygen.” Her broader point, she told me, was that
siblings, who share only about half their DNA, are as unalike as they
are similar. She said, “On our thirteenth chromosome we’re basically two
strangers.”

Micah had come with his wife, Steffi, and their ten-month-old, Hadley, a
bright, sly child with an endearingly defiant stare. As the adults sat
around talking, Hadley plotted to make off with the ramekins of almonds
and glasses of wine. Each time she evaded adult supervision and vaulted
onto the coffee table, Micah took the opportunity to troll his sister,
saying delightedly, “Looks like Hadley won the genetic lottery!” Harden
rolled her eyes and reminded him that this was the opposite of what
she’d meant. Micah, as it turned out, knew precisely what she meant; he
had already described the book to Steffi as “telling the right that they
didn’t bootstrap and telling the left that interventions are more
complicated than they want to believe,” which Harden conceded was not a
terrible précis. Micah and Steffi had met playing soccer, and Harden
teased them that Hadley might forsake the pitch for musical theatre. She
thinks that all the books about the minor decisions of parenting—whether
to introduce carrots or broccoli first, say—are “an attempt to
psychologically defend ourselves from how little control we have in the
world, about ourselves and our children.”

The episode at Russell Sage had prompted Harden to think about what her
research really meant: “The experience was a pivot point for me, away
from a career that was almost entirely about the production of empirical
research and toward doing more metascience.” “The Genetic Lottery”
reflects her years spent wandering in the desert. The book does not shy
away from technical details, but it wears its learning lightly;
alongside Harden’s frequent Biblical allusions are references to the
movies “Clueless” and “Sliding Doors.”

Two roofers and a horse on the roof of a small building.
“Now that I see him up here, I think we might not have needed the horse
at all.”
Cartoon by Zachary Kanin
Harden described her book to me as “fundamentally defensive in a lot of
ways,” and before she makes any claims for what we can learn from gwas
results she goes into great detail about their limitations. gwas simply
provides a picture of how genes are correlated with success, or mental
health, or criminality, for particular populations in a particular
society at a particular time: it wouldn’t make sense to compare findings
for educational attainment for women in America today with women who
came of age before sex-based discrimination was outlawed in higher
education. And gwas results are not “portable”: a study conducted on
white Britons tells you little about people in Estonia or Nigeria.
Polygenic scores remain poor predictors of individual outcomes—there are
plenty of people on the low end of the spectrum for educational
attainment who go on to graduate studies, and plenty of people on the
high end who never secure a high-school diploma.

gwas results can accidentally reveal as much about culture or geography
as they do about genes. A study of chopstick use in San Francisco would
find that proficiency is genetically correlated with East Asian
ancestry, which is a far cry from the discovery of an inborn dexterity
with a particular utensil. One way to sidestep this pitfall is by
comparing gwas results within families, where they have been shown to
reliably account for differences in life outcomes among siblings. But
even this measure does not solve Christopher Jencks’s redhead problem.
“A person might go far in education because they are smart and curious
and hard-working, or because they are conforming and risk-averse and
obsessive, or because they have features (pretty, tall, skinny,
light-colored) that privilege them in an intractably biased society,”
Harden writes. “A study of what is correlated with succeeding in an
education system doesn’t tell you whether that system is good, or fair,
or just.”

At some point, Harden has to set aside her caveats and assert that sheer
genetic luck plays a causal role in outcomes that matter: “If people are
born with different genes, if the genetic Powerball lands on a different
polygenic combination, then they differ not just in their height but
also in their wealth.” For her, accepting this is the necessary prelude
to any conversation about what to do about it. “If you want to help
people, you have to know what’s most effective, so you need the
science,” she told me. Harden thinks that the conversation about
behavior genetics will continue to go in circles as long as we preserve
the facile distinction between immutable genetic causes and malleable
environmental ones. We would be better off if we accepted that
everything is woven of long causal chains from genes through culture to
personhood, and that the more we understand about them the more
effective our interventions might be.

The first thing that social-science genomics can do is help researchers
control for confounding genetic variables that are almost universally
overlooked. As Harden puts it in her book, “Genetic data gets one source
of human differences out of the way, so that the environment is easier
to see.” For example, beginning in 2002, the federal government spent
almost a billion dollars on something called the Healthy Marriage
Initiative, which sought to reduce marital conflict as a way of
combatting poverty and juvenile crime. Harden was not surprised to hear
that the policy had no discernible effect. Her own research showed that,
when identical-twin sisters have marriages with different levels of
conflict, their children have equal risk for delinquency. The point was
not to estimate the effects of DNA per se, but to provide an additional
counterfactual for analysis: would an observed result continue to hold
up if the people involved had different genes? Harden can identify
studies on a vast array of topics—Will coaching underresourced parents
to speak more to their children reduce educational gaps? Does having
dinner earlier improve familial relationships?—whose conclusions she
considers dubious because the researchers controlled for everything
except the fact that parents pass along to their children both a home
environment and a genome.

She acknowledged that gwas techniques are too new, and the anxieties
about behavior genetics too deeply entrenched, to have produced many
immediately instrumental examples so far. But she pointed to a study
from last year as proof of concept. A team of researchers led by Jasmin
Wertz, at Duke, used gwas results to examine four different “aspects of
parenting that have previously been shown to predict children’s
educational attainment: cognitive stimulation; warmth and sensitivity;
household chaos (reverse-coded to indicate low household chaos); and the
safety and tidiness of the family home.” They found that one of
them—cognitive stimulation—was linked to children’s academic achievement
and their mothers’ genes, even when the children did not inherit the
relevant variants. Parental choices to read books, do puzzles, and visit
museums might be conditioned by their own genes, but they nevertheless
produced significant environmental effects.

Even the discovery that a particular outcome is largely genetic doesn’t
mean that its effects will invariably persist. In 1972, the U.K.
government raised the age at which students could leave school, from
fifteen to sixteen. In 2018, a research group studied the effects of the
extra year on the students as adults, and found that their health
outcomes for measures like body-mass index, for whatever reason,
improved slightly on average. But those with a high genetic propensity
for obesity benefitted dramatically—a differential impact that might
easily have gone unnoticed.

Some of Harden’s most recent research has looked at curricular tracking
for mathematics, an intuitive instance of how gene-environment
interactions can create feedback loops. Poor schools, Harden has found,
tend to let down all their students: those with innate math ability are
rarely encouraged to pursue advanced classes, and those who struggle are
allowed to drop the subject entirely—a situation that often forecloses
the possibility of college. The most well-off schools are able to
initiate virtuous cycles in the most gifted math students, and break
vicious cycles in the less gifted, raising the ceiling and the floor for
achievement.

Harden has perceived, in the wake of studies like these, a new
willingness to consider the role of genetics: “I get e-mails now from
curious social scientists that say, ‘I’ve never thought genetics was
useful or relevant for me, in part because I worried there was no way to
talk about genes and intelligence, or genes and behavior, without
dabbling in Murray-style scientific racism.’ ”

The Murray-Herrnstein gun that hung on the wall of William Darity’s
e-mail went off about a year later. On April 23, 2017, the popular
podcaster Sam Harris released an episode—“Forbidden Knowledge”—designed
to trigger a commotion among liberal intellectuals. Harris was
affiliated with the so-called Intellectual Dark Web, a miscellaneous
club (from which he has since distanced himself) bound together by a
shared fixation with what it perceives to be liberal groupthink. In his
interviews, Harris adopts a drowsy monotone that seems pitched to signal
his commitment to the dispassionate promotion of disputatious ideas. On
this occasion he invited listeners to “strap in” for a conversation with
Charles Murray about “The Bell Curve,” which Harris advertised as “one
of the most controversial books in living memory.”

The book generated such outsized hostility, according to Harris, because
it traffics in unpleasant truths. “People don’t want to hear that
intelligence is a real thing, and that some people have more of it than
others,” he said. “They don’t want to hear that differences in I.Q.
matter because they’re highly predictive of differential success in
life—and not just for things like educational attainment and wealth but
for things like out-of-wedlock birth and mortality. People don’t want to
hear that a person’s intelligence is in large measure due to his or her
genes and there seems to be very little we can do environmentally to
increase a person’s intelligence, even in childhood. It’s not that the
environment doesn’t matter, but genes appear to be fifty to eighty per
cent of the story. People don’t want to hear this. And they certainly
don’t want to hear that average I.Q. differs across races and ethnic
groups.”

Harris was drawn to Murray’s defense after an incident at Middlebury
College, the previous month, in which Murray was shouted down by student
protesters and his faculty chaperone was injured in a melee. Harris
considered the deplatforming “part of an anti-free-speech hysteria that
is spreading on college campuses,” and concluded, “I find the dishonesty
and hypocrisy and moral cowardice of Murray’s critics shocking. And the
fact that I was taken in by this defamation of him, and effectively
became part of a silent mob that was just watching what amounted to a
modern witch-burning—that was intolerable to me.” The two men discussed
Murray’s contention that observed racial differences are at least partly
genetic in origin, and that meliorist interventions like welfare and
affirmative-action programs are unlikely to prove successful.

Harris seemed less interested in Murray as a scholar or pundit than as a
culture-war trope. Soon after the events at Middlebury, the Web magazine
Vox had published a piece that rejected even Murray’s basic points about
intelligence tout court. Harris’s podcast seemed designed to reveal that
the left’s repudiation of Murray was motivated by politics rather than
by science. After it was released, Vox asked Turkheimer to contribute a
rebuttal, and he proposed that Harden collaborate. Harden felt a
responsibility to accept the assignment. “People are very tempted by
Murray’s ideas, and there’s a certain kind of person who almost
certainly hasn’t read ‘The Bell Curve’ but listens to Sam Harris, who
has a huge audience,” she told me.

She believed that the left’s standard-issue response was unhelpful.
“This is a very Christian thing I’m about to say, but it reminds me of
the episode where Jesus is tempted by Satan in the desert,” she told me,
in Bozeman. “There’s just enough truth in Murray that if you say, ‘This
is all wrong,’ you paint yourself into a corner where you say
intellectually dishonest things. Jesus has to say, ‘This part is true,
and this part is false.’ ” She stopped herself. “Don’t write that I’m
comparing Murray to Satan,” she said, and then continued, “I know we all
want to say it’s not true that ‘intelligence tests predict things,’ but
that’s not the lie.” To say that sort of thing ran the risk of
furthering the martyrology of Murray, and of lending lustre to the
notion that his ideas were indeed “forbidden knowledge.” The scholar and
critic Fredrik deBoer, who has drawn heavily on Harden’s work, has been
even more pointed in his criticism. In a 2017 essay, he wrote, “Liberals
have flattered themselves, since the election, as the party of facts,
truth tellers who are laboring against those who have rejected reason
itself. And, on certain issues, I suspect they are right. But let’s be
clear: the denial of the impact of genetics on human academic outcomes
is fake news.”

The Vox piece, which Harden and Turkheimer wrote with the social
psychologist Richard Nisbett, was headlined “Charles Murray is once
again peddling junk science about race and IQ.” There is a lot of good
evidence, they wrote, to support the ideas that “intelligence, as
measured by IQ tests, is a meaningful construct” and that “individual
differences in intelligence are moderately heritable.” They even
conceded, with many qualifications, that “racial groups differ in their
mean scores on IQ tests.” But there was simply no good scientific reason
to conclude that observed racial gaps were anything but the fallout from
the effects of racism. They pointed out that in the one instance when
Harris used James Flynn’s work to push back against Murray’s ideas,
Murray responded with some hand-waving about a research paper that he
admitted was too complicated for him to understand.

Despite its inflammatory headline, the article represented an unusually
subtle culture-war intervention. Nevertheless, Harris and his legion of
supporters took it as the instigation of a “smear campaign.” In
Quillette, the researcher Richard Haier compared Harden and Turkheimer’s
repudiation of Murray to climate-change denial—the second time in a year
that Harden had been thus indicted, this time from the right. The
recriminations of what Harden now describes as “the Vox fiasco” dragged
on over the next year, with parades of arguments and counterarguments,
leaked personal e-mails, and levels of sustained podcasting that were,
by anyone’s standards, extreme. Harden told me, “The popular reaction
was so divorced from that of the scientific community that men on the
Internet were sending me papers to read without realizing they were
citing work by my ex-husband, and that the work itself was a
meta-analysis of my own papers.”

Last summer, an anonymous intermediary proposed to Harris and Harden
that they address their unresolved issues. Harden appeared on Harris’s
podcast, and patiently explained why Murray’s speculation was
dangerously out in front of the science. At the moment, technical and
methodological challenges, as well as the persistent effects of an
unequal environment, would make it impossible to conduct an experiment
to test Murray’s idly incendiary hypotheses. She refused to grant that
his provocations were innocent: “I don’t disagree with you about
insisting on intellectual honesty, but I think of it as ‘both/and’—I
think that that value is very important, but I also find it very
important to listen to people when they say, ‘I’m worried about how this
idea might be used to harm me or my family or my neighborhood or my
group.’ ” (Harris declined to comment on the record for this piece.) As
she once put it in an essay, “There is a middle ground between ‘let’s
never talk about genes and pretend cognitive ability doesn’t exist’ and
‘let’s just ask some questions that pander to a virulent on-line
community populated by racists with swastikas in their Twitter bios.’ ”

Harden is not alone in her drive to fulfill Turkheimer’s dream of a
“psychometric left.” Dalton Conley and Jason Fletcher’s book, “The
Genome Factor,” from 2017, outlines similar arguments, as does the
sociologist Jeremy Freese. Last year, Fredrik deBoer published “The Cult
of Smart,” which argues that the education-reform movement has been
trammelled by its willful ignorance of genetic variation. Views
associated with the “hereditarian left” have also been articulated by
the psychiatrist and essayist Scott Alexander and the philosopher Peter
Singer. Singer told me, of Harden, “Her ethical arguments are ones that
I have held for quite a long time. If you ignore these things that
contribute to inequality, or pretend they don’t exist, you make it more
difficult to achieve the kind of society that you value.” He added,
“There’s a politically correct left that’s still not open to these
things.” Stuart Ritchie, an intelligence researcher, told me he thinks
that Harden’s book might create its own audience: “There’s so much
toxicity in this debate that it’ll take a long time to change people’s
minds on it, if at all, but I think Paige’s book is just so clear in its
explanation of the science.”

The nomenclature has given Harden pause, depending on the definition of
“hereditarian,” which can connote more biodeterminist views, and the
definition of “left”—deBoer is a communist, Alexander leans libertarian,
and Harden described herself to me as a “Matthew 25:40 empiricist” (“The
King will reply, ‘Truly I tell you, whatever you did for one of the
least of these brothers and sisters of mine, you did for me’ ”). The
political sensitivity of the subject has convinced many sympathetic
economists, psychologists, and geneticists to keep their heads below the
parapets of academia. As the population geneticist I spoke to put it to
me, “Geneticists know how to talk about this stuff to each other, in
part because we understand terms like ‘heritability,’ which we use in
technical ways that don’t always fully overlap with their colloquial
meanings, and in part because we’re charitable with each other, assume
each other’s good faith—we know that our colleagues aren’t eugenicists.
But we have no idea how to talk about it in public, and, while I don’t
agree with everything she said, sometimes it feels like we’ve all been
sitting around waiting for a book like Paige’s.”

TITLE The family that stares into the abyss together gets through
whatever insanity the year has in store together
Cartoon by Zoe Si

Harden’s outspokenness has generated significant blowback from the left.
On Twitter, she has been caricatured as a kind of ditzy bourgeois
dilettante who gives succor to the viciousness of the alt-right. This
March, after she expressed support for standardized testing—which she
argues predicts student success above and beyond G.P.A. and can help
increase low-income and minority representation—a parody account
appeared under the handle @EugenicInc, with the name “Dr. Harden, Social
Justice Through Eugenics!” and the bio “Not a determinist, but yes,
genes cause everything. I just want to breed more Hilary Clinton’s for
higher quality future people.” One tweet read, “In This House We
Believe, Science is Real, Womens Rights are Human Rights, Black Lives
Matter, News Isnt Fake, Some Kids Have Dumb-Dumb Genes!!!”

In 2018, she wrote an Op-Ed in the Times, arguing that progressives
should embrace the potential of genetics to inform education policy.
Dorothy Roberts, a professor of law, sociology, and Africana studies at
the University of Pennsylvania, strongly disagreed: “There’s just no way
that genetic testing is going to lead to a restructuring of society in a
just way in the future—we have a hundred years of evidence for what
happens when social outcomes are attributed to genetic differences, and
it is always to stigmatize, control, and punish the people predicted to
have socially devalued traits.” Darity, the economist, told me that he
doesn’t see how Harden can insist that differences within groups are
genetic but that differences between them are not: “It’s a feint and a
dodge for her to say, ‘Well, I’m only looking at variations across
individuals.’ ”

There is a good precedent for this kind of concern. In “Blueprint,”
Robert Plomin wrote that polygenic scores should be understood as
“fortune tellers” that can “foretell our futures from birth.” Jared
Taylor, a white-supremacist leader, argued that Plomin’s book should
“destroy the basis for the entire egalitarian enterprise of the last 60
or so years.” He seized on Plomin’s claim that, for many outcomes,
“environmental levers for change are not within our grasp.” Taylor
wrote, “This is a devastating finding for the armies of academics and
uplift artists who think every difference in outcome is society’s
fault.” He continued, “And, although Blueprint includes nothing about
race, the implications for ‘racial justice’ are just as colossal.”
Harden has been merciless in her response to behavior geneticists whose
disciplinary salesmanship—and perhaps worse—inadvertently indulges the
extreme right. In her own review of Plomin’s book, she wrote, “Insisting
that DNA matters is scientifically accurate; insisting that it is the
only thing that matters is scientifically outlandish.” ​(Plomin told me
that Harden misrepresented his intent. He added, “Good luck to Paige in
convincing people who are engaged in the culture wars about this middle
path she’s suggesting. . . . My view is it isn’t worth confronting
people and arguing with them.”)

With the first review of Harden’s book, these dynamics played out on
cue. Razib Khan, a conservative science blogger identified with the
“human biodiversity” movement, wrote that he admired her presentation of
the science but was put off by the book’s politics; though he notes that
a colleague of his once heard Harden described as “Charles Murray in a
skirt,” he clearly thinks the honorific was misplaced. “Alas, if you do
not come to this work with Harden’s commitment to social justice, much
of the non-scientific content will strike you as misguided, gratuitous
and at times even unfair.” This did not prevent some on the Twitter left
from expressing immediate disgust. Kevin Bird, who describes himself in
his Twitter bio as a “radical scientist,” tweeted, “Personally, I
wouldn’t be very happy if a race science guy thought my book was good.”
Harden sighed when she recounted the exchange: “It’s always from both
flanks. It felt like another miniature version of Harris on one side and
Darity on the other.”

The day after Harden’s brother returned to Memphis, she and I went for a
walk around the campus of Montana State University. We wandered into the
Museum of the Rockies, which has a world-class collection of dinosaur
fossils, and she remarked that the experience would have been more fun
with her children. I asked if her work had given her any special
insights into the challenges of parenting, and she laughed and threw up
her hands, joking that the only established public roles for psychology
professors were either as center-right pundits or as dispensers of
child-rearing advice. She told me, “As a parent, I try to keep in mind
that differences between people are examples of runaway feedback loops
of gene-by-environment interaction. People have some initial genetic
predisposition to something, and that leads them to choose certain
friends over other friends, and these initial exposures have a certain
effect, and you like that effect and you choose it again, and then these
feedback loops become self-reinforcing.”

Behavior geneticists frequently quote an old disciplinary chestnut about
how first-time parents are naïve behaviorists and that a second child
turns them into convinced geneticists. In one chapter of her book,
Harden mentions that her son struggles with a speech impairment.
“Looking at how my children differ in their ability to articulate words,
I can easily see the capricious hand of nature,” she writes. “When it
comes to inheriting whatever combination of genetic variants allows one
to pronounce a word like ‘squirrel’ by the age of three, my daughter was
lucky. My son was not.” She emphasizes that parents are already well
aware of how we might talk about genetics without making normative
judgments. “I certainly am not implying that one of my children is
‘superior’ or ‘inferior’ to the other one,” she writes. “Verbal ability
is valued, but having strong verbal ability doesn’t make one of my
children more valuable to me. The genetic differences between them are
meaningful for their lives, but those differences do not create a
hierarchy of intrinsic worth.”

The ultimate claim of “The Genetic Lottery” is an extraordinarily
ambitious act of moral entrepreneurialism. Harden argues that an
appreciation of the role of simple genetic luck—alongside all the other
arbitrary lotteries of birth—will make us, as a society, more inclined
to ensure that everyone has the opportunity to enjoy lives of dignity
and comfort. She writes, “I think we must dismantle the false
distinction between ‘inequalities that society is responsible for
addressing’ and ‘inequalities that are caused by differences in
biology.’ ” She cites research showing that most people are much more
willing to support redistributive policies if differences in opportunity
are seen as arbitrarily unfair—and deeply pervasive.

As she put it to me in an e-mail, “Even if we eliminated all
inequalities in educational outcomes between sexes, all inequalities by
family socioeconomic status, all inequalities between different schools
(which as you know are very confounded with inequalities by race), we’ve
only eliminated a bit more than a quarter of the inequalities in
educational outcomes.” She directed me to a comprehensive World Bank
data set, released in 2020, which showed that seventy-two per cent of
inequality at the primary-school level in the U.S. is within demographic
groups rather than between them. “Common intuitions about the scale of
inequality in our society, and our imaginations about how much progress
we would make if we eliminated the visible inequalities by race and
class, are profoundly wrong,” she wrote. “The science confronts us with
a form of inequality that would otherwise be easy to ignore.”

The perspective of “gene blindness,” she believes, “perpetuates the myth
that those of us who have ‘succeeded’ in twenty-first century capitalism
have done so primarily because of our own hard work and effort, and not
because we happened to be the beneficiaries of accidents of birth—both
environmental and genetic.” She invokes the writing of the philosophers
John Rawls and Elizabeth Anderson to argue that we need to reject “the
idea that America is or could ever be the sort of ‘meritocracy’ where
social goods are divided up according to what people deserve.” Her
rhetoric is grand, though the practical implications, insofar as she
discusses them, are not far removed from the mid-century
social-democratic consensus—the priorities of, say, Hubert Humphrey. If
genes play a significant role in educational attainment, then perhaps we
ought to design our society such that you don’t need a college degree to
secure health care.

In my conversations with her colleagues, Harden’s overarching idea was
almost universally described as both beautiful and hopelessly quixotic.
As one philosopher put it, “What I love about Paige, and also what I
find so incredibly moving and courageous and reckless about her, is that
she thinks she can change the whole apparatus—this large-scale framework
for moral responsibility—on the basis of our understanding of our genes.
I’m not sure genetics has the capacity to shift our intuitions, at least
on the left—because of course the right already cares about genes. In
principle, the left could try to take genes as a starting point, too,
but in practice it’s probably a different story. It’s really awful to
think about, but I think the fact that she’s an attractive and
charismatic Southern woman seems not irrelevant to her desirability as a
culture-war ally for the right.” James Tabery, a philosopher at the
University of Utah, believes that underscoring genetic difference is
just as likely to increase inequality as to reduce it. “It’s truly noble
for Paige to make the case for why we might think of biological
differences as similar to socially constructed differences, but you’re
bumping into a great deal of historical, economic, political, and
philosophical momentum—and it’s dangerous, no matter how noble her
intentions are, because once the ideas are out there they’re going to
get digested the way they’re going to get digested,” he said. “The
playing board has been set for some time.”

In Bozeman, Harden seemed anxious that she had not heard from Turkheimer
about her book. It took him a long time to get around to reading it, he
told me, in part because of the ways their ideas have diverged in recent
years, but when he finally did he wrote her an e-mail that said, “I
really do think the book is great—in fact I think it will be instantly
recognized as the most important book about behavior genetics that has
ever been written. You should get ready to be very famous.” He told me,
“I’m really proud of Paige. She’s amazing. And it’s, well, an
interesting experience to have a student that gets this successful based
in part on disagreeing with you.” He still looked askance at gwas. “I
think that Paige’s dilemma—and I don’t mean this in a bad way, because
she takes the problem very seriously—is in that balance that everyone
has to seek. If you’re me, who thinks that it’s all just correlation,
then you’re the ‘gloomy prospect’ guy and everybody thinks you’re a wet
blanket. And if you think, ‘Wow, the whole world turned out to be
genetic,’ then you’re Charles Murray, and in between you have to walk
this very careful path. You have to believe in a certain amount of
genetic causation or you don’t have a science, and you can’t believe in
too much genetic causation or you believe that poor people are poor
because they have poor genes—and that’s a very, very delicate walk.”

Harden’s political optimism is tempered by a serene personal realism. At
the end of our walk, she admitted that it wasn’t always easy to
reconcile herself with whatever it was that behavior geneticists’
results were telling us. “Take the heritability of an outcome like
divorce—it’s totally wild, because there’s a whole other person there!”
Plenty of twin research suggests a meaningful, if puzzling, genetic
correlation with divorce. Harden’s parents are divorced, as is she.

“I use this example of my sunglasses,” she said. She removed her
Ray-Bans and took out her phone to show me a photograph of two previous
pairs, both of which had lost the same lens. “I think of the
heritability of life events as the repeatability of things that seem
serendipitous. I’m clumsy in ways that persist over time, I have certain
tastes that persist over time, and I guess I think of the heritability
of divorce in the same way. My subjective experience of my sunglasses
being broken is that you have good intentions and life goes awry—it’s
easy to interpret these things as events that happen to you. But, on the
other hand, I bring all sorts of things that make these experiences
repeatable in ways that are extremely difficult to describe. It’s
obviously difficult to do exact science on the ways I repeatedly break
my Ray-Bans, just like it’s difficult or impossible to explain marital
status on a molecular level.” She picked her sunglasses up off the table
and put them back on. “But I do think that in the end you end up
becoming yourself.” ♦

New Yorker Favorites
My childhood in a cult.
What does it mean to die?
The cheating scandal that shook the world of master sommeliers.
Can we live longer but stay younger?
How mosquitoes changed everything.
Why paper jams persist.
Sign up for our daily newsletter to receive the best stories from The
New Yorker.

SubjectRepliesAuthor
o Can Progressives Be Convinced That Genetics Matters?

By: a425couple on Mon, 13 Sep 2021

0a425couple
server_pubkey.txt

rocksolid light 0.9.81
clearnet tor